ISB 6. The 6th International Symposium on Bilingualism.

June 2, 2007 - University of Hamburg

Diminutives and gender awareness in bilingual acquisition

Leonie CornipsAafke HulkSusanne Brouwer

Meertens InstituteUniversity of AmsterdamMax Planck Institute

Royal Netherlands AcademyNijmegen

@

1.Problems with the acquisition of neuter definite determiner het

Table 1: Dutch morphology of definite determiners

Definite determiners
Gender of noun / Singular / Plural
common / de / de
neuter / het / de

Production data bilingual children (Blom et al. 2006, this talk); (Hulk & Cornips 2006a,b); (Cornips & Hulk 2006); (Cornips et al. 2006):

  • Age of ultimate attainment acquisition het unclear/after age 13;
  • Quantity and quality of input seem to be crucial.

Production data monolingual children:

Van de Velde (2004), Policenska (p.c., control groups), Hulk & Cornips (2006a,b), Cornips et al. (2006), Blom et al. (2006):

  • Initially almost only de, later massive overgeneralization of de both with neuter and common nouns;
  • Het comes in slowly and late: no targetlike production before age 6 at the earliest, errors persist until a more advanced age.

This is different from what happens in the L1 acquisition process of other languages with gender morphology on the definite determiner. Especially, the difference with German is striking: in German monolingual children produce articles with the correct gender around age 3 (Mills 1986).

2. A linguistic analysis of the acquisition of grammatical gender

Hawkins & Franceschina (2004): D has an un-interpretable, grammatical gender feature which has to be erased/checked by the lexical gender feature of the noun:

-Initially children do not have this un-interpretable gender feature on D in their grammar. Their (non targetlike) use of different forms of the determiner is not based on grammatical concord with the noun, but on other strategies, related to the phonological cues;

-A point in the development comes when the grammatical u-gender feature on D is triggered – thereafter their use is targetlike.

Possible triggers according to Hawkins & Franceschina (2004):

  • Paradigmatic link between definite and indefinite articles;
  • Input above a certain threshold.

What about Dutch – what could be the trigger?

Not much evidence in the input:

  • No paradigmatic link between definite and indefinite article wrt grammatical gender, no gender on indefinite articles, no gender on plural definite articles but gender on single definite articles and demonstratives;
  • Other gender marked elements: confusing. Gender marking on pronominal elements are partially based on other features [± count, ± animate];
  • Frequency differences between neuter (25%) and common (75%) nouns.

HOWEVER

One salient trigger for grammatical gender: the diminutive suffix

In Dutch any noun, regardless of its lexical gender, “becomes” neuter in taking the diminutive suffix -(t)je:

(1)a.detafel

the (common) table

b.hettafeltje

the (NEUTER) table +DIM

c.detafel-tje-s

the (PLUR) table (DIM) PLUR

(2)a.hetboek

the (NEUTER) book

b.hetboekje

the (NEUTER) book +DIM

c.deboek-je-s

the (PLUR) book (DIM) PLUR

3.Diminutives and the acquisition of grammatical gender

3.1Experimental production data

Van de Velde (2003, 2004): monolingual children

In an experiment eliciting the definite determiner, Dutch children age 3 make significantly less errors with diminutives than with other neuter nouns; for 4 and 6 year olds, this difference is clearly present but not statistically significant.

Van Ginkel (2006:49): monolingual production: similar results

Table 2: The determiner production by monolingual children, age 6;0 – 7;6, for neuter nouns and diminutives in an elicitationtask (picture description task based on Blom 2006), target in grey

production / monolingualsn=8
age / 6;0-7;6
neuter nouns / diminutives
de / 3940.6 % / 1914.8 %
het / 4749 % / 10380.5 %
een ‘a’ / 1010.4 % / 64.7 %
Ø / 00 % / 00 %
total / 96100 % / 128100 %

Table 3: The determiner production by monolingual children, age 10;3 – 11;5, for neuter nouns and diminutives in an elicitationtask (picture description task based on Blom 2006), target in grey

production / monolingualsn=8
age / 10;3-11;5
neuter nouns / diminutives
de / 1515.6 % / 64.7 %
het / 7679.2 % / 11791.4 %
een ‘a’ / 44.2 % / 43.1 %
Ø / 11 % / 00 %
total / 96100 % / 128100 %

HOWEVER,

Van Ginkel (2006:49): bilingual – Turkish-Dutch production results

Table 4: The determiner production by Turkish-Dutch children, age 6;0 – 7;6, for neuter nouns and diminutives in an elicitationtask (picture description task based on Blom 2006), target in grey

production / bilingual – turkish-dutchn=10
age / 6;0-7;6
neuter nouns / diminutives
de / 6352.5 % / 7546.9 %
het / 5545.8 % / 7748.1 %
een ‘a’ / 10.8 % / 31.9 %
Ø / 10.8 % / 42.5 %
missing / 00 % / 10.6 %
total / 12099.9 % / 160100 %

Table 5: The determiner production by Turkish-Dutch children, age 10;0-11;10, for neuter nouns and diminutives in an elicitationtask (picture description task based on Blom 2006), target in grey

production / bilingual – turkish-dutchn=11
age / 10;0-11;10
neuter nouns / diminutives
de / 5340.2% / 5833%
het / 7758.3% / 11464.8%
een ‘a’ / 21.5% / 21.1%
Ø / 00% / 10.6%
missing / 00% / 10.6%
total / 132100% / 176100.1%

The production data of the bilingual children in Table 4 & 5 differ from the monolingual results in Table 2 & 3.

QUESTIONS

Is there awareness of grammatical gender at all? More specifically, do the bilingual children recognize the ‘diminutive’ as trigger for grammatical gender?

4. Knowledge awareness experiments

4.1 Experimental design (see Brouwer 2006)

An experimental design is set up to “tap the knowledge” in monolingual and bilingual children with respect to their gender system of definite determiners of diminutives. Spoken items are paired with an identical image on the screen of a laptop monitor. The subjects have to decide as soon as possible whether the spoken item is correct or incorrect in Dutch. They responded by pressing buttons on a button box. One button had a happy smiley face that the subjects pressed to indicate they believed the sentence to be correct Dutch, and another button had a sad smiley face that the subjects pressed if they believed the sentence to be incorrect Dutch.

After the practice session, two lists with each 32 spoken experimental items, are offered (see table 6).Every experimental item consists of correct: Ik zie het hondje ‘I see theN dog-DIMN; andincorrect: Ik zie *de hondje ‘I see theC dog- DIMN. In other words, each presented diminutive is paired with a correct and an incorrect definite determiner. Moreover, a distinction is made between 8 common and 8 neuter nouns.

Table 6: Overview of all presented items

Grammatical form / Ungrammatical form / Total
Practice items / 3 / * 3 / 6
Experimental items / 16 / * 16 / 32
common noun / 8 / * 8 / 16
e.g. / Ik zie het lepeltje / * Ik zie de lepeltje
‘I see the spoonC-DIM’ / * ‘I see the spoonC-DIM’
neuter noun / 8 / * 8 / 16
e.g. / Ik zie het paardje / * Ik zie de paardje
‘I see the horseN-DIM’ / * ‘I see the horseN-DIM’
Fillers / 4 / * 4 / 8

Examples:

Ik zie het hondje; *Ik zie de hondjeIk zie het schaapje; *Ik zie de schaapje

‘I see the dog-common-DIM’‘I see the sheep-neuter-DIM’

4.2Results diminutives (Van Ginkel 2006)

Table 7: Accuracy rates (% correct) of *DE+DIM and HET+DIM by monolingual children in two age groups, target in grey

input / L1
n=8 / L1
n=8
age / 6;0-7;6 / 10;3-11;5
correct judgment / incorrect judgment / correct judgment / incorrect judgment
correct
HET+DIM / 116
90.6% / 12
9.4% / 123
96.1% / 5
3.9%
incorrect
DE+DIM / 56
43.7% / 72
56.3% / 97
75.8% / 31
24.2%

Table 8: Accuracy rates (% correct) of DE+DIM and HET+DIM by bilingual Turkish-Dutch children in two age groups, target in grey

input / bilingual
n=10 / bilingual
n=11
age / 6;0-7;6 / 10;3-11;5
correct judgment / incorrect judgment / correct judgment / incorrect judgment
correct
HET+DIM / 145
90.6% / 15
9.4% / 153
86.9% / 23
13.1%
incorrect
DE+DIM / 26
16.2% / 34
83.8% / 96
54.5% / 80
45.5%

•Bilinguals of both age groups reveal much lower accuracy rates for incorrect *DE+DIM than monolinguals; high accuracy rates for correct HET+DIM may be due to ‘yes’-strategy (cf Brouwer et al.); Question: is diminutive a trigger?

However:

•Bilinguals reveal development between age groups 6-7;6 and 10;3-11;5. The accuracy-rates for incorrect *DE+DIM increase from 16.2% to 54.5%, respectively.

5.Discussion

•bilingual children age 6;0 – 7;6

-do not produce more correct het with diminutives (45.8%) than non-diminutives (48.1%; see Table 4);

-are not ‘aware’ of neuter gender with respect to diminutives (accuracy rate of incorrect *de + diminutives is 16.2 %, see Table 8).

•bilingual children age 10;3 -11;5

-produce some more correct het with diminutives (64.8%) than with non-diminutives (58.3%; see Table 5);

-show some ‘awareness’ of neuter gender with respect to diminutives (accuracy rate of incorrect *de + diminutives is 54.5 %, see Table 8). Although 54.5% is around chance level, it’s a much higher percentage than in the younger bilingual group (16.2%).

JUST DELAY?

However, with respect to the acquisition of neuter gender in Dutch, even 11-13 years old bilinguals are still non-targetlike and significantly different from monolinguals of the same age both in production (see Cornips et al. 2006) and ‘awareness’ (see Brouwer et al. 2006).

QUESTION

•Do bilinguals need another trigger than diminutives? OR

•Do they fossilize?

References

Berkum, J.J.A. van (1996). The psycholinguistics of grammatical gender. Studies in

language comprehension and production. Nijmegen: Nijmegen University

Press.

Blom, E. (2005). Dutch adjectival inflection: L1, child L2 and adult L2 learners. Talk

presented at the Radboud University.

Blom, E., D. Polisenská & F. Weerman. (2006). Effects of age in the acquisition of

gender: a three-way distinction between child L1, child L2 and adult L2 acquisition. Talk presented at Amsterdam Gender Colloquium, Vrije Universiteit, 16 September 2006.

Bol, G.W., and Kuiken, F. (1988). Grammaticale analyse van taalontwikkelingsstoornissen. Phd. Dissertation. University of Amsterdam.

Brouwer, S. (2006). An experimental study about possible dissociation between

representation and processing definite determiners of L1 and child L2 Dutch in

different sociolinguistic contexts. Trainee report Meertens Institute/University of Utrecht.

Brouwer, S., L. Cornips & A. Hulk. (2007). Misrepresentation of Dutch neuter gender in older bilingual children? ms. University of Utrecht, Meertens Instituut & University of Amsterdam.

Cornips, L. & A. Hulk. (2006). Bilingual and bidialectal language development:

grammatical gender in Dutch. Lefebvre, C., L. White & C. Jourdan (eds).The

Montreal Dialogues. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Cornips, L., M. van der Hoek & R. Verwer (2006). The acquisition of grammatical

gender in bilingual child acquisition of Dutch (by older Moroccan and Turkish

children). The definite determiner, attributive adjective and relative pronoun. Linguistics-in-The Netherlands, 2006. B. Los & J. van de Weijer (eds): 40-51.

Gillis, S., Schaerlaekens, A.M. (eds.). Kindertaalverwerving. Een handboek voor het

Nederlands. Groningen: Martinus Nijhoff.

Ginkel, B. van (2006). “De verwerving van lexicaal en morfologisch bepaald geslacht. Een onderzoek naar tweede taalverwerving bij eentalige Nederlandse kinderen en tweetalige Turks-Nederlandse kinderen”. Master thesis Meertens Instituut/University of Utrecht.

De Houwer, A. and S. Gillis. 1998. The acquisition of Dutch. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

Hawkins, Roger & Florencia Franceschina. (2004). “Explaining the acquisition and non- acquisition of determiner-noun gender concord in French and Spanish”. The acquisition of French in Different Contexts ed. by P. Prévost & J. Paradis, 175–205. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

Hulk, A. to appear. Deviance in early child bilingualism. LSRL

Hulk, A. & L. Cornips (2006a) ‘Neuter gender and interface vulnerability in child

L2/2L1 Dutch. In: Sharon, U. et. al. (eds.), Paths of Development in L1 and L2

acquisition: In honor of Bonnie D. Schwartz. 2006. p. 107–134.’

Hulk, A. & L. Cornips. (2006b). Between 2L1 - and child L2 acquisition: an

experimental study of bilingual Dutch. Interfaces in Multilingualism:

Acquisition and representation. Hamburg Studies on Multilingualism Vol 4.

Amsterdam: John Benjamins. C. Lleo (ed), pp. 115-137.

Hulk, A. C. J. and N. Müller 2000. Bilingual first language acquisition at the interface

between syntax and pragmatics'. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 3 (3):

227-244.

Meisel, J. 1989. "Early differentiation of language in bilingual children." In

Bilingualism across a lifespan: Aspects of acquisition, maturity and loss, K. Hyltenstam and L. Obler (eds.), 13-40. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Mills, A.E. (1986). Mills, A. (1986) The acquisition of gender. A Study of English and German. Berlijn-Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag,.

Müller, N. A. Hulk 2001. "Cross-linguistic influence in bilingual first language

acquisition: Italian and French as recipient languages." Bilingualism:

Language and Cognition 4 (1): 1-21.

Sabourin, L.L., and Haverkort, M. (2003). Neural substrates of representation and

processing of a second language. In: R. van Hout, A. Hulk, F. Kuiken, R.

Towell (eds.). The lexicon-syntax interface in second language acquisition.

Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Sabourin, L.L. (to appear). Grammatical gender agreement in L2 processing.

Research Proposal. University of Groningen.

Unsworth, S. (2006). "On the acquisition of noun gender in 2L1A/child L2 Dutch."

Talk presented at the Linguistic in the Netherlands day, 4 February.

Unsworth, S. (in press). Age and input in early child bilingualism: The acquisition of grammatical gender in Dutch.’ In A. Belikova, L. Meroni and M. Umeda (eds.). Galana 2- Proceedings of the Conference on generative Aprroaches to langzame Acquisition North America 2. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.

Velde, M. van der (2004). L'acquisition des determinants en L1: une etude

comparative entre le Francais et le Néerlandais. Acquisition et interaction en

langue etrangere, 21, 9 - 46.

Velde, M. van der (2002). Determiner acquisition in Dutch and French. TIN-dag

(Linguistics in the Netherlands). University Utrecht.

1